• Deutsch
Login

Open Access

  • Home
  • Search
  • Browse
  • Publish
  • FAQ
  • Institutes

Deutsches Institut für Internationale Pädagogische Forschung (DIPF)

Refine

Author

  • Hasselhorn, Marcus (12)
  • Schmiedek, Florian (10)
  • Linkersdörfer, Janosch (8)
  • Lonnemann, Jan (8)
  • Lindberg, Sven (7)
  • Goldhammer, Frank (6)
  • Drachsler, Hendrik (4)
  • Fiebach, Christian (4)
  • Shing, Yee Lee (4)
  • Brod, Garvin (3)
+ more

Year of publication

  • 2020 (14)
  • 2018 (8)
  • 2013 (5)
  • 2019 (5)
  • 2012 (4)
  • 2014 (4)
  • 2017 (4)
  • 2015 (2)
  • 2010 (1)
  • 2016 (1)
+ more

Document Type

  • Article (43)
  • Book (2)
  • Conference Proceeding (1)
  • Contribution to a Periodical (1)
  • Doctoral Thesis (1)
  • Preprint (1)

Language

  • English (43)
  • German (6)

Has Fulltext

  • yes (49)

Is part of the Bibliography

  • no (49)

Keywords

  • Behavior (3)
  • PISA (3)
  • elementary school (3)
  • selective attention (3)
  • ADHD (2)
  • Motivation (2)
  • Multimodal Learning Analytics (2)
  • cognitive development (2)
  • development (2)
  • episodic memory (2)
+ more

Institute

  • Deutsches Institut für Internationale Pädagogische Forschung (DIPF) (49)
  • Psychologie (37)
  • Erziehungswissenschaften (7)
  • Informatik (4)
  • Präsidium (3)
  • Medizin (1)
  • Neuere Philologien (1)
  • Psychologie und Sportwissenschaften (1)
  • studiumdigitale (1)

49 search hits

  • 1 to 10
  • 10
  • 20
  • 50
  • 100

Sort by

  • Year
  • Year
  • Title
  • Title
  • Author
  • Author
"Wenig zielführender Alarmismus" (2017)
Hartig, Johannes
DIPF-Bildungsforscher Johannes Hartig antwortet auf die Kritik von Hans Peter Klein am »Kompetenztaumel« im deutschen Bildungssystem
Robust reconstruction of curved line structures in noisy point clouds (2021)
Ritter, Marcel ; Schiffner, Daniel ; Harders, Matthias
Point-based geometry representations have become widely used in numerous contexts, ranging from particle-based simulations, over stereo image matching, to depth sensing via light detection and ranging. Our application focus is on the reconstruction of curved line structures in noisy 3D point cloud data. Respective algorithms operating on such point clouds often rely on the notion of a local neighborhood. Regarding the latter, our approach employs multi-scale neighborhoods, for which weighted covariance measures of local points are determined. Curved line structures are reconstructed via vector field tracing, using a bidirectional piecewise streamline integration. We also introduce an automatic selection of optimal starting points via multi-scale geometric measures. The pipeline development and choice of parameters was driven by an extensive, automated initial analysis process on over a million prototype test cases. The behavior of our approach is controlled by several parameters — the majority being set automatically, leaving only three to be controlled by a user. In an extensive, automated final evaluation, we cover over one hundred thousand parameter sets, including 3D test geometries with varying curvature, sharp corners, intersections, data holes, and systematically applied varying types of noise. Further, we analyzed different choices for the point of reference in the co-variance computation; using a weighted mean performed best in most cases. In addition, we compared our method to current, publicly available line reconstruction frameworks. Up to thirty times faster execution times were achieved in some cases, at comparable error measures. Finally, we also demonstrate an exemplary application on four real-world 3D light detection and ranging datasets, extracting power line cables.
A boon and a bane: comparing the effects of prior knowledge on memory across the lifespan (2019)
Brod, Garvin ; Shing, Yee Lee
We tested 6–7-year-olds, 18–22-year-olds, and 67–74-year-olds on an associative memory task that consisted of knowledge-congruent and knowledge-incongruent object–scene pairs that were highly familiar to all age groups. We compared the three age groups on their memory congruency effect (i.e., better memory for knowledge-congruent associations) and on a schema bias score, which measures the participants’ tendency to commit knowledge-congruent memory errors. We found that prior knowledge similarly benefited memory for items encoded in a congruent context in all age groups. However, for associative memory, older adults and, to a lesser extent, children overrelied on their prior knowledge, as indicated by both an enhanced congruency effect and schema bias. Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging (fMRI) performed during memory encoding revealed an age-independent memory x congruency interaction in the ventromedial prefrontal cortex (vmPFC). Furthermore, the magnitude of vmPFC recruitment correlated positively with the schema bias. These findings suggest that older adults are most prone to rely on their prior knowledge for episodic memory decisions, but that children can also rely heavily on prior knowledge that they are well acquainted with. Furthermore, the fMRI results suggest that the vmPFC plays a key role in the assimilation of new information into existing knowledge structures across the entire lifespan. vmPFC recruitment leads to better memory for knowledge-congruent information but also to a heightened susceptibility to commit knowledge-congruent memory errors, in particular in children and older adults.
Epistemic network analyses of economics students’ graph understanding: an eye-tracking study (2020)
Brückner, Sebastian ; Schneider, Jan ; Zlatkin-Troitschanskaia, Olga ; Drachsler, Hendrik
Learning to solve graph tasks is one of the key prerequisites of acquiring domain-specific knowledge in most study domains. Analyses of graph understanding often use eye-tracking and focus on analyzing how much time students spend gazing at particular areas of a graph—Areas of Interest (AOIs). To gain a deeper insight into students’ task-solving process, we argue that the gaze shifts between students’ fixations on different AOIs (so-termed transitions) also need to be included in holistic analyses of graph understanding that consider the importance of transitions for the task-solving process. Thus, we introduced Epistemic Network Analysis (ENA) as a novel approach to analyze eye-tracking data of 23 university students who solved eight multiple-choice graph tasks in physics and economics. ENA is a method for quantifying, visualizing, and interpreting network data allowing a weighted analysis of the gaze patterns of both correct and incorrect graph task solvers considering the interrelations between fixations and transitions. After an analysis of the differences in the number of fixations and the number of single transitions between correct and incorrect solvers, we conducted an ENA for each task. We demonstrate that an isolated analysis of fixations and transitions provides only a limited insight into graph solving behavior. In contrast, ENA identifies differences between the gaze patterns of students who solved the graph tasks correctly and incorrectly across the multiple graph tasks. For instance, incorrect solvers shifted their gaze from the graph to the x-axis and from the question to the graph comparatively more often than correct solvers. The results indicate that incorrect solvers often have problems transferring textual information into graphical information and rely more on partly irrelevant parts of a graph. Finally, we discuss how the findings can be used to design experimental studies and for innovative instructional procedures in higher education
Cognitive prerequisites for generative learning: why some learning strategies are more effective than others (2020)
Breitwieser, Jasmin ; Brod, Garvin
This study examined age‐related differences in the effectiveness of two generative learning strategies (GLSs). Twenty‐five children aged 9–11 and 25 university students aged 17–29 performed a facts learning task in which they had to generate either a prediction or an example before seeing the correct result. We found a significant Age × Learning Strategy interaction, with children remembering more facts after generating predictions rather than examples, whereas both strategies were similarly effective in adults. Pupillary data indicated that predictions stimulated surprise, whereas the effectiveness of example‐based learning correlated with children’s analogical reasoning abilities. These findings suggest that there are different cognitive prerequisites for different GLSs, which results in varying degrees of strategy effectiveness by age.
Only some attempts at meaning making are successful: the role of change‐relatedness and positive implications for the self (2020)
Huang, Manxia ; Schmiedek, Florian ; Habermas, Tilmann
Objective: Although meaning making and specifically autobiographical reasoning are expected to relate to well‐being, findings tend to be mixed. Attempts at meaning making do not always lead to meaning made. We aimed to disentangle these complex relationships and also explore the role of level of education. Method: Ninety participants (mean age 36.73 years, SD = 7.27; 74.4% women, 25.6% men) who had experienced the loss of a parent through death, going missing, or Alzheimer's disease narrated this loss, a sad, a turning point, and a self‐defining memory, and completed questionnaires assessing depression, trauma symptoms, and protracted grief. Three aspects of autobiographical reasoning (quantity, valence, and change‐relatedness of self‐event connections) were related to meaning made (sophistication of meaning making) and symptom level. Results: Years of education correlated both with positive implications of autobiographical reasoning and with meaning made. The quantity, positivity, and change‐relatedness of attempts at meaning making (self‐event connections) predicted accomplished meaning made, and positivity alone predicted less prolonged grief. Conclusions: Adapting the life story after a loss such that change of the self is acknowledged and positive change can be constructed helps finding meaning and lowering protracted grief. These changes in narrative identity are supported by more years of education.
Being proven wrong elicits learning in children – but only in those with higher executive function skills (2019)
Brod, Garvin ; Breitwieser, Jasmin ; Hasselhorn, Marcus ; Bunge, Silvia A.
This study investigated whether prompting children to generate predictions about an outcome facilitates activation of prior knowledge and improves belief revision. 51 children aged 9–12 were tested on two experimental tasks in which generating a prediction was compared to closely matched control conditions, as well as on a test of executive functions (EF). In Experiment 1, we showed that children exhibited a pupillary surprise response to events that they had predicted incorrectly, hypothesized to reflect the transient release of noradrenaline in response to cognitive conflict. However, children's surprise response was not associated with better belief revision, in contrast to a previous study involving adults. Experiment 2 revealed that, while generating predictions helped children activate their prior knowledge, only those with better inhibitory control skills learned from incorrectly predicted outcomes. Together, these results suggest that good inhibitory control skills are needed for learning through cognitive conflict. Thus, generating predictions benefits learning – but only among children with sufficient EF capacities to harness surprise for revising their beliefs.
The role of planning in complex problem solving (2018)
Eichmann, Beate ; Goldhammer, Frank ; Greif, Samuel ; Pucitec, Liene ; Naumann, Johannes
Complex problem solving (CPS) is a highly transversal competence needed in educational and vocational settings as well as everyday life. The assessment of CPS is often computer-based, and therefore provides data regarding not only the outcome but also the process of CPS. However, research addressing this issue is scarce. In this article we investigated planning activities in the process of complex problem solving. We operationalized planning through three behavioral measures indicating the duration of the longest planning interval, the delay of the longest planning interval and the variance of intervals between each two successive interactions. We found a significant negative average effect for our delay indicator, indicating that early planning in CPS is more beneficial. However, we also found effects depending on task and interaction effects for all three indicators, suggesting that the effects of different planning behaviors on CPS are highly intertwined.
Exploring behavioural patterns during complex problem-solving (2020)
Eichmann, Beate ; Greiff, Samuel ; Naumann, Johannes ; Brandhuber, Liene ; Goldhammer, Frank
In this explorative study, we investigate how sequences of behaviour are related to success or failure in complex problem‐solving (CPS). To this end, we analysed log data from two different tasks of the problem‐solving assessment of the Programme for International Student Assessment 2012 study (n = 30,098 students). We first coded every interaction of students as (initial or repeated) exploration, (initial or repeated) goal‐directed behaviour, or resetting the task. We then split the data according to task successes and failures. We used full‐path sequence analysis to identify groups of students with similar behavioural patterns in the respective tasks. Double‐checking and minimalistic behaviour was associated with success in CPS, while guessing and exploring task‐irrelevant content was associated with failure. Our findings held for both tasks investigated, from two different CPS measurement frameworks. We thus gained detailed insight into the behavioural processes that are related to success and failure in CPS.
Verhaltenskodex für Trusted Learning Analytics : Entwurf für die hessischen Hochschulen (2020)
Hansen, Jan ; Rensing, Christoph ; Herrmann, Oliver ; Drachsler, Hendrik
Dieser Entwurf eines Verhaltenskodex richtet sich an Hochschulen, die mittels Learning Analytics die Qualität des Lernens und Lehrens verbessern wollen. Der Kodex kann als Vorlage zur Erstellung von organisationsspezifischen Verhaltenskodizes dienen. Er sollte an Hochschulen, die Learning Analytics einführen wollen, durch Konsultationen mit allen Interessengruppen überprüft und an die Ziele sowie die bestehende Praxis innerhalb der jeweiligen Hochschulen angepasst werden. Der Kodex wurde auf Grundlage einer Analyse bestehender europäischer Kodizes (Engelfriet, Manderveld & Jeunink, 2017; Westerlaken, Manderveld & Jorna, 2019; Sclater & Bailey, 2015; Open University UK, 2014; University of Edinburgh, 2018) und der in Deutschland geltenden Rechtsgrundlage vom Innovationsforum Trusted Learning Analytics des hessenweiten Projektes „Digital gestütztes Lehren und Lernen in Hessen“ entwickelt.
  • 1 to 10

OPUS4 Logo

  • Contact
  • Imprint
  • Sitelinks